|
Big numbers,
hard calculations, and little heads
A phenomenon currently of great interest to
those in cognitive development is the mental representations
of large numbers, and the relationships between these representations. Number
is an excellent example of an overarching cognitive structure,
which can be systematically broken down and studied by way
of each contributing field of cognitive science. The
ability to represent large amounts (and, critically, the relationship in
memory between these quantities) has driven entire theories
of animal learning in behavioral neuroscience. In even
the most basic models of reasoning, estimating probability
(and hence, ratios) is integral to the organisms' decisions
to act or not to act, to infer or not to infer. It is
well known that for all animals, including adult humans, differing
schedules of reinforcement produce differing rates of responding,
which must be computed by comparing the likelihood of reinforcement
with the total number of trials lapsed. The ability to compare
two amounts and abstract their ratio is possibly the anchor
of reasoning. In this talk, I will describe several lines of
research on the capacity for numerical reasoning in early infancy,
the parallels of this work with work done in animals, and possible
ways to integrate these findings into our educational systems.
There appear to be two main systems that infants
use for representing number- one for small numbers, and one
for large numbers. The small-number system is used for
1-4 items and is utilized mainly when an organism must track
objects in their environment. It is very exact and contains
a lot of information about the featural properties of these
objects (such as size, shape, and color). As the bulk
of my work (and the bulk of work done with animals) has focused
on the other, large-number, system, that is what I will be
discussing today. Both the animal and adult literature
suggest that the representation of large quantities is mediated
by a preverbal analog model of magnitude representation. This
model was originally developed to account for perceptual and
numerical competencies in rats, and subsequently proposed as
underlying humans' approximate large number representation
. It postulates an accumulator mechanism composed of: a sensory
source for a stream of impulses, a pulse former which gates
this stream of impulses to an accumulator for a fixed duration
(around 200 ms) whenever an object or event is counted, an
accumulator which sums the impulses gated to it, and a mechanism
which moves the magnitude from the accumulator to memory when
the last object has been counted. This accumulator is
subject to psychophysical laws, namely Weber's fraction limit,
which states that the variability of perception of magnitude
increases with the amount to be represented (i.e., representation
becomes more approximate and discrimination between quantities
less exact as the magnitude in the accumulator increases.) Two
processes have been hypothesized regarding this accumulator
output. Estimator processes refer to the simple production
of mental representations of number (called numerons.) Operator
processes are processes such as addition and subtraction, in
which multiple numerons are manipulated to produce another
numeron.
This topic is not completely unexplored in
human infants, as Spelke and colleagues have looked at large
number estimator processes in infants and found they can discriminate
16 from 32, and 8 from 16, but not 16 from 24 or 8 from 12
(until around 9 months of age). They conclude that this “number
sense” is present in infancy, but has a long way to go before
reaching adult–like levels of quantity discrimination of 1.15
: 1. A different set of theoretically interesting questions
arises when we look at the nature of the representation and
the type of information moved from discrimination to storage.
Specifically, I have studied the proficiency
of operator processes across development. I currently
have research which suggests that infants can perform operations
analogous to addition and subtraction over large amounts of
objects. 9-month-olds look longer to incorrect outcomes of
the large-number addition and subtraction problems 5+5 (=10
or 5), 10-5 (=10 or 5), 4+5 (=9 or 6), and 10-4 (=9 or 6). Critically,
the incorrect and correct outcomes differ by a particular ratio
(such as 2:1 in the 10 and 5 object outcomes, and 3:2 in the
9 and 6 object outcomes.) Interestingly, this proficiency
in addition and subtraction, which accords to the ratio of
the outcomes, appears to be equivalent to their ability to
simply discriminate two amounts. The operations shown
to the infants appear to add little (or no) error to the final
representations of the outcomes. I am currently extending
this work in two ways. First, I am examining the “breakdown
point” by giving infants operations whose outcomes are indiscriminable
even in a pure comparison case (such as 12 versus 9 objects.) If
infants fail at this task, then we can take steps towards concluding
that an approximate magnitude mechanism was underlying their
representation of these amounts.
Next, I am conducting an intermodal addition
and subtraction study. In this study 5 objects are moved
behind a screen, and then 5 “clunks” (which indicate the addition
of more objects) are heard by the infants. If infants
look longer to the incorrect outcome of 5 objects (and shorter
to 10 objects), then it stands to reason that they were inferring
a particular amount from the audio stimuli, and adding that
amount to the visual stimuli.
Another operation (which is of great interest
to me, perhaps because it has been largely ignored in the infant
literature) is the seemingly complicated yet ubiquitous one
of division, alluded to in the beginning of this talk. In
what is known as rate-of-return, animals in a naturalistic
feeding situation are able to spontaneously compute ratios
by dividing amount of food with the area in which that food
is found (i.e., 50 apples per tree here, and 10 apples per
tree there) and return to feed at levels similar to that ratio
(around 85% of time return for the first batch, 15% for the
second.) . The relevant question in light of this finding,
therefore, is not whether infants are capable of comparing
two quantities of items in a discrimination task, but whether
they can be “pushed up” a level to discriminating two ratios
of items. If so, this could be considered analogous to
rate-of-return. There has been no systematic study
in infancy of this crucial ability, which is offered by some
evolutionary theorists as the reason amount representation
evolved in the first place.
Here at Yale we have found very good evidence
that infants are capable of this ratio abstraction. When
habituated to a sequence of scenes displaying either 2:1 ratios
of object type x: object type y (such as 20:10, 8:4, 32:16)
or 4:1 ratios of these objects (such as 20:5, 40:20,
12:6), and then tested with either a new ratio or a new exemplar
of the old ratio (so, both a set of new 2:1 and 4:1 scenes),
children look differentially to these two types of test ratios
and this looking time will switch as a function of habituation
group. We appear to have found evidence for these ‘higher-order'
representations in the infant's memory store. The next
condition we ran controlled for extraneous perceptual cues
(such as confounds with area, or contour length), and infants
were still able to discriminate the two ratios presented, indicating
that they were computing these ratios over an abstract representation
of number itself, and not just perceptual cues associated with
number of items.
There are many potential educational directions
to take this area of study. I propose a multi-step process
which will strengthen children's representations of number,
incorporation of carefully controlled displays to the children's
texts and games, and a set of games which tap into children's
inherent sense of large number and the operations that can
be done with this intuition. To strengthen the representation
of number, one would pair sights and sounds with the identical
number being played or displayed at the same time. So,
for instance, 5 drumbeats played as 5 trees come up on screen. Sometimes
these objects would be big, sometimes little, sometimes the
same, and sometimes different. By varying the specific
perceptual aspects of both the sights and the sounds, the children
may better learn that 5 means 5, no matter what it looks or
sounds like, as an entity unto itself. The games would
be based on the videos used by many researchers who study nonverbal
operations, with the children watching as a group and then
taking turns to give a sticker to a display which showed the
correct outcome (to the addition or subtraction problem) or
the same ratio (after seeing many examples of this ratio before). By
incorporating the fun of games and scientifically created stimuli
which taps into our natural, animal-like processes we may be
able to enhance the child's educational experience.
This line of research starts to address the
broader issue of how we process and organize information from
our environment, how this activity changes across development,
and how we can use this natural way of processing to add to
our educational resources. By informing our infancy research
with established phenomena in the animal literature, we are
closer to discovery of the “fundamentals” of our nature. As
this talk indicates, my passion has come to lie at ferreting
out the shared junctures which reflect the development of cognition
throughout evolutionary history. The representation of
number is a fascinating, prime candidate through which to do
so. |